Here’s a table that summarizes the similarities and differences between an experimental and a quasi-experimental study design:
Experimental Study (a.k.a. Randomized Controlled Trial) | Quasi-Experimental Study | |
---|---|---|
Objective | Evaluate the effect of an intervention or a treatment | Evaluate the effect of an intervention or a treatment |
How participants get assigned to groups? | Random assignment | Non-random assignment (participants get assigned according to their choosing or that of the researcher) |
Is there a control group? | Yes | Not always (although, if present, a control group will provide better evidence for the study results) |
Is there any room for confounding? | No (although check Manson et al. for a detailed discussion on post-randomization confounding in randomized controlled trials) | Yes (however, statistical techniques can be used to study causal relationships in quasi-experiments) |
Level of evidence | A randomized trial is at the highest level in the hierarchy of evidence | A quasi-experiment is one level below the experimental study in the hierarchy of evidence [source] |
Advantages | Minimizes bias and confounding | – Can be used in situations where an experiment is not ethically or practically feasible – Can work with smaller sample sizes than randomized trials |
Limitations | – High cost (as it generally requires a large sample size) – Ethical limitations – Generalizability issues – Sometimes practically infeasible | Lower ranking in the hierarchy of evidence as losing the power of randomization causes the study to be more susceptible to bias and confounding |
A quasi-experimental design is a non-randomized study design used to evaluate the effect of an intervention. The intervention can be a training program, a policy change or a medical treatment.
Unlike a true experiment, in a quasi-experimental study the choice of who gets the intervention and who doesn’t is not randomized. Instead, the intervention can be assigned to participants according to their choosing or that of the researcher, or by using any method other than randomness.
Having a control group is not required, but if present, it provides a higher level of evidence for the relationship between the intervention and the outcome.
Examples of quasi-experimental designs include:
An experimental design is a randomized study design used to evaluate the effect of an intervention. In its simplest form, the participants will be randomly divided into 2 groups:
Randomization ensures that each participant has the same chance of receiving the intervention. Its objective is to equalize the 2 groups, and therefore, any observed difference in the study outcome afterwards will only be attributed to the intervention – i.e. it removes confounding.
(for more information, I recommend my other article: Purpose and Limitations of Random Assignment).
Examples of experimental designs include:
Although many statistical techniques can be used to deal with confounding in a quasi-experimental study, in practice, randomization is still the best tool we have to study causal relationships.
Another problem with quasi-experiments is the natural progression of the disease or the condition under study — When studying the effect of an intervention over time, one should consider natural changes because these can be mistaken with changes in outcome that are caused by the intervention. Having a well-chosen control group helps dealing with this issue.
So, if losing the element of randomness seems like an unwise step down in the hierarchy of evidence, why would we ever want to do it?
This is what we’re going to discuss next.
The issue with randomness is that it cannot be always achievable.
So here are some cases where using a quasi-experimental design makes more sense than using an experimental one:
I am Georges Choueiry, PharmD, MPH, and PhD student in epidemiology. I created this website to help researchers conduct studies from concept to publication. You can find me on LinkedIn.